HOPE-GM INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM
"HOPE-GM LECTURES ON PRIMATE
MIND and SOCIETY"
Organization: Primate Research Institute, Kyoto
University
Date: 22-23 March 2010, Kyoto,Japan
Place: International Conferrence Hall (2nd floor in
Clock Tower Centennial Hall)
TIMETABLES
Monday 22nd March
09:00-10:00 Registration
10:00-10:10 Opening remarks
SESSION 1 Chaired by M Tomonaga
10:10-10:30 Satoshi Hirata Auditory and visual
event-related potentials in an
awake chimpanzee
10:30-10:50 Takahisa Matsusaka Vocal communication
of captive chimpanzees
10:50-11:10 Yumi Yamanashi Assessing the effects
of cognitive experiments on the welfare of captive chimpanzees by
direct comparisons of the activity budget between wild and
captivity
11:10-11:30 Misato Hayashi Cognitive development
in chimpanzees assessed by object manipulation
11:30-12:00 Susana Carvalho From pounding to
knapping: how chimpanzee archaeology can help us model lithic
technology
12:00-13:00 Lunch
13:00-14:30 Poster session
SESSION 2 Chaired by I Adachi
14:30-15:00 Kathelijne Koops The effect of ecology on the use of
elementary technology in foraging and nest-building in the
chimpanzees of the Nimba mountains, Guinea
15:00-15:30 Kimberley Hockings Fission-fusion
dynamics in chimpanzees at Bossou, Republic of Guinea: ecological
constraints in an anthropogenic environment
15:30-15:50 Yamato Tsuji An effect of yearly
difference in nut fruiting on foraging success of female Japanese
macaques (Macaca fuscata) through within-troop contest-type
competition
15:50-16:20 Paco Bertolani Adoption of an infant
chimpanzee by an unrelated lactating mother in Tai National Park,
Cote d’Ivoire
16:20-16:40 Chie Hashimoto Male philopatry and
possible fusion of wild bonobo unit-groups at Wamba, D.R. Congo
16:40-17:40 William McGrew Fifty years of wild
chimpanzee tool use: where do we stand?
18:00-20:00 Party
Tuesday 23rd March
SESSION 3 Chaired by M Hayashi & T Imura
09:00-09:20 Shiro Kohshima
09:20-09:40 Fumihiro Kano A comparative
eye-tracking study in chimpanzees and humans
09:40-10:00 Tomoko Imura Differences in visual
temporal integration on object recognition between chimpanzees and
humans
10:00-10:20 Ikuma Adachi Direct comparison between
humans and chimpanzees for their pitch-luminance mapping
10:20-10:40 Takaaki Kaneko A comparative study in
the perception of self-agency between human and chimpanzee
10:40-11:10 Anna Albiach Serrano They bring the
food, but do they know how? Causal knowledge of strip-pulling
tasks in great apes and corvids
11:10-11:30 Masaki Tomonaga Going ahead:
Perceptual bias for forward-facing motion in chimpanzees
11:30-12:00 Sonja Koski Chimpanzee personality
assessed by observational quantification of behaviour
12:00-13:00 Lunch
SESSION 4 Chaired by S Carvalho & K Hockings
13:00-13:20 Chris Martin Shared matching-to-sample
tasks in Chimpanzees
13:20-13:40 Yuko Hattori Extracting and expressing
referential information by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
13:40-14:00 Masayuki Tanaka Acquisition of
numerical sequences in three primate species, Pan troglodytes,
Hylobates lar, and Mandrillus sphinx
14:00-14:20 Jae Choe “From so simple a
beginning” Animal cognition in Korea: primates and magpies
14:20-14:40 Sanha Kim Korea’s first field
study of primates: the behavior and ecology of wild Javan gibbons
(Hylobates moloch)
14:40-15:00 Coffee break
15:00-15:30 Malini Suchak Capuchin monkeys
cooperate with strangers: learning the benefits of reciprocity
with in-group and out-group members
15:30-15:50 Shinya Yamamoto Chimpanzees’
ability for cumulative culture: invention, modification, and
social learning of tool-use technique
15:50-16:50 Frans de Waal Prosocial primates: the
altruism question
16:50-17:00 Closing remarks
SPEAKERS’ ABSTRACTS
SATOSHI HIRATA
Great Ape Research Institute, Hayashibara
Biochemical Laboratories, Japan
Email: hirata@gari.be.to
Auditory and visual event-related potentials
in an awake chimpanzee
A series of experiments were conducted to measure
event-related potentials (ERPs) in an awake chimpanzee. In the
first study, ERPs to auditory stimuli were measured with reference
to a welldocumented component of human studies, namely mismatch
negativity (MMN). The results confirmed a MMN-like component in a
chimpanzee for the first time, implying that chimpanzees and
humans share cognitive and neural processing for detection of
deviant stimuli. In the second study, ERPs were measured for each
of the following auditory stimuli: vocal sound of subject's own
name, familiar name of other group member, unfamiliar name, and
non-vocal sound.
Following the stimulus onset, a negative shift at approximately
500ms latency was observed, in
particular with response to the subject's own name. Such specific
ERP patterns suggest that a chimpanzee processes her name
differently from other sounds. The last study revealed brain
activities in response to various kinds of pictures which are
assumed to differ in affective valances for a subject. Following
the stimulus onset, ERPs differentiated at approximately 250 ms
with regard to stimulus type, indicating that the chimpanzee
starts to process affective valance from an early stage.
TAKAHISA MATSUSAKA
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: matsusak@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Vocal communication of captive chimpanzees
Wild chimpanzees are known to live in a
"fission-fusion" society. The members of a unit-group
do not always travel together but often split into temporary
"parties" of various size and
composition. They use long-distance vocalizations such as
pant-hoots to communicate with
others out of sight. Captive chimpanzees cannot travel freely in a
forest, but chimpanzees at
Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, also show similar
patterns of fission-fusion
grouping in their daily life. They can walk around some indoor
rooms and outdoor enclosures,
and some of them also travel around several experimental rooms.
Members of the party, that
share one enclosure or room, are flexible. The purpose of this
study is to clarify how they use
vocalizations to communicate with others out of sight. To this
aim, I recorded the vocal
behaviors of chimpanzees in outdoor enclosures, and examined how
chimpanzees in the experimental rooms reacted to these
vocalizations. I used video-recorded data collected in the
experimental rooms for these analyses. I will show preliminary
results in the presentation.
YUMI YAMANASHI; MISATO HAYASHI; TETSURO
MATSUZAWA
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: yamanash@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Assessing the effects of cognitive experiments on the welfare
of captive chimpanzees by
direct comparisons of the activity budget between wild and
captivity
We investigated the effects of cognitive experiments on chimpanzee
activity budgets through
direct comparisons of wild and captive individuals. One goal of
captive management is to ensure that the activity budgets of
captive animals are as similar as possible to their wild
counterparts.
However in practice this has rarely been achieved. We compared
activity budgets among three groups of chimpanzees; wild
chimpanzees in Bossou (Guinea, N=10), captive chimpanzees who
participate in cognitive experiments (participant chimpanzees,
N=6), and those who don’t participate in the experiments
(non-participant chimpanzees, N=6) at Primate Research Institute
(Kyoto University). Data from captivity were obtained from both
experimental days (weekdays) and non-experimental days (weekends).
In both wild and captive situations, the first author collected
data using focal animal sampling and recorded behaviours every one
minute. The results show that during weekdays, feeding and resting
times of participant chimpanzees were almost the same as wild
chimpanzees, whereas those of non-participant chimpanzees were
significantly different. In contrast, during weekends, feeding and
resting times of both groups of captive chimpanzees were
significantly different from wild chimpanzees. These results
suggest that cognitive experiments work as an efficient tool for
feeding enrichment.
MISATO HAYASHI
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: misato@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Cognitive development in chimpanzees assessed by object
manipulation
Primates including humans share the manual skill for sophisticated
object manipulation. Objectmanipulation tasks conducted in a
face-to-face situation were applied to both chimpanzees and humans
as a comparative scale of cognitive development. Blocks of
different shapes were given to chimpanzees to test their physical
understanding in a stacking-block context. The subjects were
required to selectively use the appropriate orientation to stack
up blocks efficiently. The performance in chimpanzees was comparable to that in humans of 2-3 years of age. The nestingcup
task illuminated the fundamental similarity between chimpanzees
and humans in making a hierarchical combination among multiple
objects. Analysis on object manipulation can also be expanded to
studies of wild chimpanzees. The behavior of chimpanzees in Bossou
was analyzed in terms of different levels of complexity and
efficiency. In sum, cognitive development in captive and wild
chimpanzees was examined nonverbally by focusing on the patterns
of their object manipulation.
SUSANA CARVALHO1; TETSURO MATSUZAWA2;
WILLIAM MCGREW1
1 LEVERHULME CENTRE FOR HUMAN EVOLUTIONARY STUDIES, UNIVERSITY OF
CAMBRIDGE
2 PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: scr50@cam.ac.uk
From pounding to knapping: How chimpanzee archaeology can help
us model lithic technology
Understanding how and why tool-use emerged in primates has been a
persisting goal of archaeology and anthropology. From the
archaeological record, stone tool technology is thought to be at
least 2.6 million years old. Features of knapped stones have
allowed archaeologists to discriminate between different lithic
artefacts, i.e. intentionally modified stones. However, it has not
been possible to directly associate the earliest tool-user in the
human lineage with the Oldowan technology, as the oldest tools are
not directly associated with fossils. Lithic industries were
described as a continuum, progressing from the most simple to the
most complex. However, the discovery and identification of hominin
species that are thought to exploit different ecological niches
simultaneously, suggests that Homo habilis may have lost the title
of “the first tool-maker”. Integration of these data
and the evolution of lithic technologies has yet to be done. In
this paper, we present data on chimpanzee nut-cracking at Bossou,
Guinea, West Africa, and use non-human primate models to ascertain
if older pounding tool industries might be unrecognised in the
archaeological record. We present an overview of primate
archaeology, focusing on the emergence of tool-use, a behavioural
complex that dramatically increases the efficiency of food
processing, and probably played a crucial role as an evolutionary
selective force. Raw material-selectivity, discrimination of tool
function, emergence of possessiveness,
and preferential re-use of the same tool elements may have
facilitated the first incidental
knapping episodes.
KATHELIJNE KOOPS
LEVERHULME CENTRE FOR HUMAN EVOLUTIONARY STUDIES, UNIVERSITY OF
CAMBRIDGE
Email: kk370@cam.ac.uk
The effect of ecology on the use of elementary technology in
foraging and nest-building in
the chimpanzees of the nimba mountains, Guinea
Elementary technology denotes the knowledgeable use of one or more
physical objects as a
means to achieve an end. Today we know little about the effects of
ecological conditions on the
use of elementary technology. The question that presents itself is
essential to the understanding
of the evolution of material culture: How does the environment
affect the use of elementary
technology? The aim of my Ph.D. research is to characterize the
environmental factors that
influence the use of elementary technology in foraging and in
shelter construction (i.e. nestbuilding)
among unhabituated chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in the
Nimba Mountains,
Guinea, West Africa. The following questions are addressed: 1)
Which forms of elementary
technology in foraging are present in the Nimba chimpanzees (e.g.
nut cracking, termite fishing,
ant dipping) and do they vary seasonally? 2) Is the use of
elementary technology in foraging
related to the temporal or spatial availability of target species
(e.g. nuts, termites, ants) or
appropriate tool materials? 3) Is the use of elementary technology
in foraging related to the
temporal or spatial availability of other food sources (e.g.
fruit)? In addition, I address the
question: what is the function of nest-building, both in trees and
on the ground?
KIMBERLEY J HOCKINGS1; JAMES R ANDERSON2; TETSURO
MATSUZAWA3
1 NEW UNIVERSITY OF LISBON, PORTUGAL
2 UNIVERSITY OF STIRLING, SCOTLAND
3 PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: hock@fcsh.unl.pt
Fission-fusion dynamics in chimpanzees at Bossou, Republic of
Guinea: ecological
constraints in an anthropogenic environment
The ecological-constraints model can be used to test assumptions
related to how animals modify
their socio-ecology to environmental changes and predicts that
when within group feeding
competition is high (i.e. when habitat-wide food availability is
low) groups should split into
smaller sub-groups to reduce ranging costs. Chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes verus) inhabiting a
forest-farm mosaic at Bossou, Republic of Guinea, did not adapt
their party size to changes in
wild fruit abundance. Instead other factors such as presence of a
maximally swollen female and
consortship behaviour in combination with cultivated resource
consumption were important
determinants of party size. Chimpanzees at Bossou actually moved
more when wild fruits were
abundant but both sexes adapted their activity budgets in
different ways to integrate guarded
crops into their broader ecological strategy. Although it is
probable that the Bossou chimpanzee
community is characterized by a reduced degree of fission-fusion
dynamics than other
communities due to its small size, these results also emphasise
that short-term changes in party
sizes and activity budgets can be at least partly attributed to a
specific set of environment and
social conditions, including those associated with proximity of
the village and high levels of
exposure to local people. With species being increasingly forced
into anthropogenically impacted
habitats, access to energy-rich crops must be incorporated into
models that are traditionally used
to explain fission-fusion dynamics and other socio-ecological
adaptations to more natural
environments.
YAMATO TSUJI
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: ytsuji@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
An effect of yearly difference in nut fruiting on foraging success
of female Japanese
macaques (Macaca fuscata) through within-troop contest-type
competition
We studied the effects of annual differences in nut-fruiting in
four nut-producing tree species on
the foraging success of adult female Japanese macaques through
intra-group contest-type
competition on Kinkazan Island, northern Japan. Among the four
tree species, only T. nucifera
nut fruited in 2004, while the nuts of all species, especially F.
crenata nut, fruited in 2005. The
Torreya trees were less abundant and small-sized, while Fagus
crenata trees were more
abundant and large-sized. We therefore expected that intra-troop
contest-type competition among
female macaques would be more severe in 2004 than 2005. As we
expected, antagonistic
behavior occurred more frequently in 2004. Furthermore, in 2004
high ranking macaques
obtained a greater daily metabolizable energy intake, while the
low ranking macaques could not
fulfill their energy requirement by nut feeding alone. In 2005, in
contrast, energy intakes of all
female macaques from nut species exceeded their energy
requirements and inter-rank differences
in foraging behavior were not found. Thus, inter-annual difference
in energy production and the
distribution of the main foods affected their foraging success.
This would consequently affect
population parameters like survival rate and birth rate through
intra-group contest-type
competition.
PACO BERTOLANI
LEVERHULME CENTRE FOR HUMAN EVOLUTIONARY STUDIES, UNIVERSITY OF
CAMBRIDGE
Email: mpb44@cam.ac.uk
Adoption of an infant chimpanzee by an unrelated lactating mother
in Tai National Park,
Cote d’Ivoire
The extent to which wild chimpanzees truly exhibit altruistic
behaviours is often debated, for
example through analyses of food sharing and cooperative
engagements. However the fitness
costs to the donor are often difficult to quantify except through
analyses of long-term
reproductive success. Here we present data on one case of an adult
female chimpanzee at Tai
National Park in Cote d’Ivoire simultaneously nursing and
transporting two male infants, one her
biological son and the other an adopted infant. Genetic analyses
confirmed that the adult female
was not the mother of the adopted infant and the adopted infant
exhibited behavioural traits
commonly seen in orphan chimpanzees; he was more independent with
regards to feeding and
travelling but engaged in more resting and less play behaviour
than the other infant. Although
adoption is known to occur in both wild and captive chimpanzees,
this is the first report of spontaneous and complete adoption of a dependant infant that
includes breast feeding by the
adopted mother. Given the direct fitness costs for the adopter
(and presumably to the biological
son) and the survival benefits for the adoptee, we explore the
possible reasons for this behaviour.
CHIE HASHIMOTO1, YASUKO TASHIRO2, EMI HIBINO1, MBANGI
MULAVWA3,
KUMUGO YANGOZENE3, TAKESHI FURUICHI1, GEN’ICHI
IDANI4, OSAMU
TAKENAKA1
1 PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY, JAPAN
2 GREAT APE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, HAYASHIBARA BIOCHEMICAL
LABORATORIES, JAPAN
3 RESEARCH CENTER FOR ECOLOGY AND FORESTRY, MINISTRY OF SCIENTIFIC
RESEARCH, D.R. CONGO
4 WILDLIFE RESEARCH CENTER OF KYOTO UNIVERSITY, JAPAN
Email: hashimot@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Male philopatry and possible fusion of wild bonobo unit-groups at
Wamba, D.R. Congo
We report on the possible fusion of a wild bonobo unit-group at
Wamba, D.R. Congo. Research
at Wamba started in 1973 but due to civil war was interrupted from
1996 to 2001. When research
restarted in 2001 some individuals could not be visually
identified. Based on the results of DNA
analyses and direct observations, at least two adult males and two
adult females were confirmed
to have entered the study group (E1 group) during or following the
war. In the areas surrounding
the home range of E1 group, two groups of bonobos (Kofola and
Bokela groups) disappeared
and the home range of E1 group expanded into these areas. This
might be better understood as
the aggregation of declining groups, rather than a strict
intergroup transfer of adult males.
Bonobos sometimes display affiliative behaviors towards members of
different groups and
individuals from different groups sometimes forage together for
week long periods. This
between-group tolerance in bonobos might have enabled the
permanent aggregation of
fragmented groups.
WILLIAM MCGREW
LEVERHULME CENTRE FOR HUMAN EVOLUTIONARY STUDIES, UNIVERSITY OF
CAMBRIDGE
Email: wcm21@cam.ac.uk
Fifty years of wild chimpanzee tool use: where do we stand?
In 1960, Jane Goodall reported the first habitual use of tools by
wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), thus opening up the field of primate elementary
technology. Since then, there have
been many advances, both empirical and practical, reviewed here
for the last five years.
Following an anthropology analogy, studies of chimpanzee
technology have gone through stages
of natural history, ethnography, and ethnology. At least 8 field
sites offer fully-habituated
subjects. Results from these sites (and others) show chimpanzees
to be unique in their material
culture, amongst all living non-human species. Neither capuchin
monkeys nor corvids, nor any
other great ape species, present such a range of technological
complexity. Analyses now go
beyond simple presence/absence of tool use/making. Areas of
special interest are: tool kit, tool
set, tool composite, compound tool, technology without tools, and
primate archaeology. New
forms of tool use continue to be discovered, and known forms
continue to be refined. Despite
claims to the contrary, studies of living apes, especially the
chimpanzee, provide the best models
for understanding the evolutionary origins of human technology,
through collaboration between
primatologists and palaeo-anthropologists.
SHIRO KOHSHIMA1, HIROMI KOBAYASHI2, NOKO
KUZE1
1 WILDLIFE RESEARCH CENTER, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
2 FACULTY OF HUMAN-ENVIRONMENT STUDIES, KYUSHU UNIVERSITY
Email: kohshima@wrc.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Face and eye morphology of the human and nonhuman primates:
implications in visual
communication
Human eyes have a widely exposed white sclera surrounding the
darker colored iris, making it
easy to discern the direction in which they are looking. We
compared the external morphology of
primate eyes in nearly half of all primate species, and show that
this feature is uniquely human.
In addition, humans have the largest ratio of exposed sclera in
the eye outline, which itself is
most elongated horizontally. We suggest that these two features
are adaptations to extend the
visual field by allowing greater eye movement, especially in the
horizontal direction.
Comparison of eye coloration and facial coloration around the eye
suggested that the dark
coloration of exposed sclera of nonhuman primates is an adaptation
to camouflage the gaze
direction against other individuals and/or predators, and that the
white sclera of the human eye is
an adaptation to enhance the gaze signal. We will also discuss
implications of facial morphology
in visual communication by comparing developmental changes and
sexual differences of facial
morphology in great apes.
FUMIHIRO KANO1,2; MASAKI TOMONAGA1
1 PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
2 JAPAN SOCIETY FOR PROMOTION OF SCIENCE
Email: fkanou@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
A comparative eye-tracking study in chimpanzees and humans
We introduce a novel approach to comparative cognition studies
―a comparative eye-tracking
study in chimpanzees and humans. Eye-tracking methodology enables
us to compare the eye
movements of two species directly (i.e. both qualitatively and quantitatively). Firstly, we show
the striking similarities in eye movements between chimpanzees and
humans when viewing
photographs. Both species freely viewed the same sets of
photographs depicting bodies and faces
under the same experimental conditions. The similarities between
the species were pronounced
in terms of how they view faces. Secondly, we focus on the general
differences in eye
movements between chimpanzees and humans by presenting various
scenes (i.e. both social and
non-social scenes). Previous studies of humans showed that
different eye movement patterns
play different roles in semantic processing. In this study, humans
exhibited longer durations of
fixation on average and spent more time viewing objects/faces in
peripheral vision compared to
chimpanzees. We interpret these results in terms of
species-specific strategies in eye
movement/information processing.
TOMOKO IMURA
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: imura@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Differences in visual temporal integration on object recognition
between chimpanzees and
humans
According to observations of wild and captive chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes), individuals seem
to be able to recognize an object that is partially hidden by
obstacles (e.g. tree branches). Such
competence should be related with the ability to convert local
fragmented visual features into a
complete image. However, some studies report that chimpanzees
often show difficulty when
trying to convert these partial visual features. This study
reexamined the ability to integrate
partial elements into a whole image, using four chimpanzees and
eight humans, as subjects. A
line drawing that represents an object was moved behind a slit (6,
18, or 30 pixels in width) at
either a slow or fast speed, followed by three line drawings
presented on a monitor screen. One
of the three drawings was identical to a drawing that previously
moved behind the slit. The task
was to choose this same drawing amongst the three alternatives.
Results indicated that humans
had high accuracy in all speeds and in all slit-width conditions.
In contrast, with chimpanzees,
the rate of correct responses decreased during the high speed or
the narrowest slit conditions.
These findings suggest that chimpanzees are able to recognize an
object by converting a
fragment into a complete image, but their ability to do so is quantitatively different from humans.
IKUMA ADACHI1, VERA LUDWIG2, TETSURO
MATSUZAWA1
1PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
2DEPARTMENT OF PSYCHIATRY, UNIVERSITY OF BONN
Email: adachi@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Direct comparison between humans and chimpanzees for their
pitch-luminance mapping
Findings from developmental psychology have shown that human
toddlers are already able to
associate high pitch sounds, rather than low pitch sounds, with
lighter colors. It has been argued
that the tendency to systematically match visual and auditory
dimensions was a driving factor in
the evolution of language. However, no one has yet addressed the
crucial issue if non-human
animals experience cross-modal correspondences as we do. Here we
provide the first direct
comparison between humans and chimpanzees on their pitch-luminance
mapping. Participants
from both species were required to classify squares as black or
white, while hearing irrelevant
background sounds that were either high-pitched or low-pitched.
Chimpanzees made more
mistakes when the background sound was synaesthetically
incongruent (low-pitched for white,
high-pitched for black) than when it was synaesthetically
congruent (high-pitched for white, lowpitched
for black). In humans, the effect was evident through increased
latencies in incongruent
trials in line with previous research. These results suggest that
such cross-modal correspondence
is shared in these two species and such cross-modal associations
reflect evolutionary old
mechanisms in the primate brain.
TAKAAKI KANEKO1,2; MASAKI TOMONAGA1
1 PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
2 JAPAN SOCIETY FOR PROMOTION OF SCIENCE
Email: tkaneko@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
A comparative study in the perception of self-agency between human
and chimpanzee
Humans perceive an event that they cause differently from other
events that occur in their
environment. Such unique experiences, accompanied by our voluntary
actions, are called the
sense of self-agency and allow us to establish the concept of self
as being an independent agent.
Here, we studied how chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), our
evolutionary closest neighbors,
perceive the effect of own voluntary action and compared with that
of humans. We developed a
task to conduct a comparative study between species. In the
experiment, two cursors were shown
on a computer monitor, one of which was the distractor cursor
moved by the computer and the
other was the self-cursor controlled by the participant using the
trackball. The participants were
required to detect the self-cursor. We found that chimpanzees have
the cognitive capacity to
correctly identify the cursor which they could control based on
the spatial and temporal
congruence between one’s own action and its effect. We also
found chimpanzees were more
dependent on the goal of action rather than kinematic motion for
the self-other distinction and
suggest some discontinuities between humans and chimpanzees in the
perception of self-agency.
ANNA ALBIACH-SERRANO
MAX PLANCK INSTITUTE FOR EVOLUTIONARY ANTHROPOLOGY, GERMANY
Email: anna.albiach@eva.mpg.de
They bring the food, but do they know how?
Causal knowledge of strip-pulling tasks in great apes and corvids
Great apes are known to be flexible tool users, but causal
knowledge should not be assumed:
individuals might be responding to perceptual cues instead.
Corvids have also shown complex
tool use. Whether this similarity in behavior reflects the use of
similar cognitive mechanisms is
another question that remains to be answered. We used a
strip-pulling paradigm where subjects
had to choose between two strips, one holding a reward. First, we
presented members of the 4
non-human great ape species with both a classical strip-pulling
task (causal task) and another perceptually similar task that lacked the causal relation among
its elements (perceptual task).
Apes solved the causal but not the perceptual task. Second, we
presented the same problem to
two corvid species, the Common raven (Corvus corax) and the
Carrion crow (Corvus corone).
Corvids only solved the easier patterns of the causal task.
Finally, we presented a similar task to
Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), Chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes) and 2,
3 and 4 year-old children (Homo sapiens). All species solved the causal task, but only 4
year-olds solved the perceptual
task. Our results do not support the perceptual hypothesis and
show different knowledge of strippulling
tasks in great apes and corvids.
MASAKI TOMONAGA
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: tomonaga@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Going ahead: Perceptual bias for forward-facing motion in
chimpanzees
When a horizontal row of shapes presented in the window is shifted
laterally, we humans
perceive apparent motion with either the leftward or the rightward
direction. This ambiguous
direction becomes unambiguous if the shape itself has an
“intrinsic” direction: we perceive these
directional shapes move “forward”. This implies that
some kind of “knowledge” established
through the daily experiences affect the perceptual judgments in
humans. If our closest neighbor,
chimpanzees, exhibited similar kind of perceptual biases for
forward-facing motion, we can use
this bias as a tool for probing their knowledge in the various
domain. In the current study, two
young chimpanzees judged the direction of 2-frame motion display
(apparent motion) of the
directional triangles, photographs of chimpanzee quadrupedal
walking, 3/4-view faces of
chimpanzees and humans, etc. For non-biological but directional
stimuli (triangle), chimpanzees
exhibited no bias for the “forward” facing motion.
Chimpanzee walking and 3/4-view faces,
however, caused very clear perceptual bias for forward facing
motion. Furthermore, additional
experiment showed that this was not simply the discrimination of
stimulus direction itself;
forward-facing bias disappeared when the apparent motion was not
presented. These results
imply that chimpanzees also show the perceptual bias for
forward-facing motions, but the effect
was limited to biological agents. They may recognize which is
“forward” of the chimpanzee
body and expect chimpanzee moves the direction to which the head
is orienting.
SONJA KOSKI
LEVERHULME CENTRE FOR HUMAN EVOLUTIONARY STUDIES, UNIVERSITY OF
CAMBRIDGE
Email: sek39@cam.ac.uk
Chimpanzee personality assessed by observational quantification of
behaviour
Personality, or individual differences in behaviour and underlying
psychology, has rapidly
become an active field of behavioural, psychological and
evolutionary research. Animal
personality research can be divided into
‘psychological’ and ‘biological’
approaches, each
employing a different set of questions and methods. Chimpanzee
personality has mostly been
studied with the psychological approach, which utilises a
theoretical framework, trait selection
process and evaluation methods used in human personality research.
This work has described
chimpanzee personality structure as largely similar to that of
humans. However, we know little
of chimpanzee personality at the behavioural level. I study
personality of captive chimpanzees
employing the biological approach. The candidate personality
traits are selected from a range of
naturally occurring behaviours in ecologically meaningful
categories. Preliminary results show that behavioural traits are sufficiently repeatable, informative
of chimpanzee personality
structure and useful in investigations of personality’s
influence on individual fitness. The results
will inform of key personality traits in chimpanzees at the
behavioural level and of their
structural organisation, both of which are relevant for
understanding the foundations of
personality. I discuss my research in the theoretical framework of
integrating the behavioural and
psychological approaches to animal personality.
CHRIS MARTIN
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: martin@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Shared Matching-To-Sample Tasks in Chimpanzees
In the current study, we modified a traditional computerized
Match-to-Sample (MTS) paradigm
such that two chimpanzees could participate in a single task,
providing us with a novel window
onto socio-cognitive processes. In our task, input from both
subjects was necessary to complete
MTS tasks, with one subject (the model) performing the first half
of the trial, and the other (the
observer) completing the trial using the model's actions as
discriminative cues. Subjects took
turns being models and observers, and were given a series of both
symbolic and non-symbolic
MTS tasks. Performance on social matching tasks was compared to
that attained by the same
subjects on identical MTS tasks but without the social element
(i.e. performed individually). We
found that both subjects were able to use the cues provided by a
conspecific model to complete
non-symbolic MTS tasks, and one of the two subjects, named Ai, was
able to complete symbolic
MTS while using the model's touch cue to determine the sample's
identity. Ai displayed a
significant decrease in performance when required to combine
social cueing and symbolic
representation processes. Our study establishes a novel paradigm
for examining social
interactions and social learning within a highly controlled and
automated setting.
YUKO HATTORI
PRIMATE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: yhattori@pri.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Extracting and expressing referential information by chimpanzees (Pan
troglodytes)
For animals living in highly social communities, reading
information of others and conveying
information to others are very important communicative skills.
Here I introduce two experiments
concerning referential communication by chimpanzees. Using an eye
tracking technique, the first
experiment shows that static images of a conspecific’s
actions modulate chimpanzees looking
behaviors more effectively than those of an allospecific’s
actions, whereas humans are readily
sensitive to social cues of both species. In addition to this,
increased first looking duration to a
conspecific’s face suggests that chimpanzees extract more
information from conspecific faces
than those of allospecifics. In the second experiment, we used a
natural feeding situation and
found that chimpanzees changed their begging gestures more
flexibly when they begged for food
directly held by a human experimenter but such flexibility
disappeared when they begged for
food placed on a table. These results suggest that chimpanzees are
better able to understand
others’ actions than humans, and the need for chimpanzees to
convey their desires referentially
affects the flexibility of their gestures. Moreover, previous
studies that test social cognition in
nonhuman primates using human stimuli might have underestimated
their abilities.
MASAYUKI TANAKA
WILDLIFE RESEARCH CENTER, KYOTO UNIVERSITY
Email: mtanaka@wrc.kyoto-u.ac.jp
Acquisition of numerical sequences in three primate species, Pan
troglodytes, Hylobates lar,
and Mandrillus sphinx
Comparative cognitive studies on three primate species are being
conducted at the Kyoto City
Zoo. All experiments are open to visitors. The subjects include
four chimpanzees, a pair of
gibbons, and a family of four mandrills. All individuals were
previously naive to cognitive
testing. The task used involves serial learning using Arabic
numerals. Each experiment with the
gibbons or the mandrills took place in the outdoor compound. An
experimenter located on the
other side of the wire fence presented the subjects with a
touch-sensitive monitor. Experiments
with chimpanzees were held in an experimental booth adjacent to
the indoor compound of the
chimpanzees. Two sets of touch-sensitive monitors, computers, and
universal feeders were
installed in the booth. None of the subjects were isolated or
separated during the experiment, and
therefore, all had equal opportunity to touch the screen and
initiate the task. While all
chimpanzees readily interacted with the touch-sensitive monitors,
the gibbons and the mandrills
needed prior training to touch the monitor. Currently, an adult
male gibbon and two young
mandrills participate in the task. Each subject is progressively
learning to perform the task. This
study reveals no significant differences in the learning rate and
curve across the three species.
JAE CHOE
EWHA WOMANS UNIVERSITY, SEOUL, KOREA
Email: jaechoe@ewha.ac.kr
“From So Simple a Beginning”
Animal Cognition in Korea: Primates and Magpies
A dream to establish a primate research program in Korea began
when I met Jane Goodall and
Tetsuro Matsuzawa more than a decade ago. Then I came to a 2002
international primatological
conference in Inuyama and announced a quite audacious but rather
uncertain plan for primate
research in Korea. In the following years students from my lab
were given opportunities to have
valuable learning sessions at the PRI weeks at a time. I myself
have also visited Max Planck
Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology and Marc Hauser’s
lab at Harvard University as well as
the PRI to seek wider perspectives and advice. Based on these
experiences we launched a field
study of Javan gibbons at Gunung Halimun-Salak National Park,
Indonesia, in 2007. We have
successfully collected two-year data on foraging ecology and are
now conducting field studies on
acoustic communication and mating systems. In collaboration with
Seoul Zoo, we are also in the
process of setting up a PRI-style cognitive science laboratory. My
Magpie Team which has been
carrying out long-term ecological research (LTER) for over 13
years has also begun a series of
cognition experiments. This talk will introduce the history and
future plan for the study of animal
cognition in Korea. From such a simple beginning endless
possibilities most beautiful and most
wonderful have been, and are being, evolved.
SANHA KIM1; SUSAN LAPPAN2,3; JAE
CHOE3
1SEOUL NATIONAL UNIVERSITY, SEOUL, KOREA,
2APPALACHIAN STATE UNIVERSITY, BOONE, NORTH CAROLINA, USA
3EWHA WOMANS UNIVERSITY, SEOUL, KOREA
Email: sanhakim@hotmail.com
Korea’s first field study of primates: the behavior and
ecology of wild Javan gibbons (Hylobates moloch)
Gibbons are arguably the least studied member of apes despite
their wide distribution throughout
Asia. In order to contribute to primatology as a newcomer and
address pressing conservation
issues, we chose the endangered Javan gibbon as our subject for
long-term field research. We
observed 3 habituated gibbon groups in the Gunung Halimun-Salak
National Park, Indonesia,
from June 2007 to March 2009, and collected behavioral and feeding
data from April 2008 to
March 2009. The gibbons were usually active from ~6:00-16:00 h and
mean daily path length
was 1180.5 m. Mean home range size was 36.6 ha, with a mean
overlapping area of 3 ha. Fruits
comprised 61.3% of the diet, while young leaves and flowers were
23.1% and 11.6%,
respectively. However, fruit feeding time was unrelated to overall
fruit abundance and gibbons
showed preference among fruit species. Gibbon behavior differed
when feeding on preferred and
non-preferred fruits in terms of revisit frequency and pattern.
The difference in feeding bout
length between subsequent visits was significantly larger for
preferred species, and bout length
was longer in later visits for preferred but not non-preferred
species. This suggests that gibbons
organize their daily foraging schedule around preferred food
sources.
MALINI SUCHAK
NATIONAL PRIMATE RESEARCH CENTER, EMORY UNIVERSITY, USA
Email: msuchak@emory.edu
Capuchin Monkeys Cooperate with Strangers:
Learning the Benefits of Reciprocity with In-Group and Out-Group
Members
Humans are thought to be the only species capable of widespread
cooperation with unrelated
individuals. The purpose of this study was to determine if learned
reciprocity facilitates
cooperation in situations where cooperation normally breaks down
in nonhuman primates:
inequity aversion and among strangers. Twelve brown capuchin
monkeys (Cebus apella) were
tested to determine if they could learn the benefits of reciprocal
exchange in a prosocial choice
task. Capuchins did not develop contingent reciprocity in this
task. Instead, mutualism led to an
increase in prosocial behavior during reciprocal situations.
Furthermore, capuchins overcame
inequity aversion through reciprocity and demonstrated widespread
cooperation with unfamiliar
monkeys. These results demonstrate that (1) simple cognitive
mechanisms can lead to increased
cooperation and (2) cooperation outside the social group is not
limited to humans.
SHINYA YAMAMOTO1,2,3; GEN
YAMAKOSHI4; TANYA HUMLE5;
MASAYUKI TANAKA6; TETSURO MATSUZAWA7
1JSPS POSTDOC RESEARCH FELLOW,
2UNIVERSITY OF TOKYO, TOKYO, JAPAN,
3GREAT APE RESEARCH INSTITUTE, HAYA-SHIBARA, OKAYAMA, JAPAN,
4ASAFAS, KYOTO UNIVERSITY, KYOTO, JAPAN,
5UNIVERSITY OF KENT, CANTERBURY, UK,
6WRC, KYOTO UNIVERSITY, KYOTO, JAPAN,
7PRI, KYOTO UNIVERSITY, INUYAMA, JAPAN
Email: yamamoto@darwin.c.u-tokyo.ac.jp
Chimpanzees’ ability for cumulative culture: invention,
modification, and social learning of
tool-use technique
It has been suggested that only humans show cumulative culture,
with successive generations
building on earlier achievements. Here we present two studies
which investigated chimpanzees’
capacity for cumulative culture. The first is the observation of a
new tool-use behavior in a wild
chimpanzee in Bossou, Guinea. At this site, ant-fishing in trees
had never been observed in over
27 years, although ant-dipping on the ground is customary. In the
first observation of ant-fishing,
the chimpanzee employed wands of similar length to those used for
ant-dipping. Two years later,
his tools for ant-fishing were shorter and more suitable for the
targeted ants. This suggests the
chimpanzee generalized ant-fishing behavior on the ground to the
trees, and adjusted his tools
accordingly. The second is an experimental study showing that
captive chimpanzees can learn a
more efficient tool-use technique by observing a skilled
conspecific. Chimpanzees changed their
technique when drinking juice with a straw, from a dipping
technique to a more efficient sucking
technique, after social observation. These two studies indicate
that chimpanzees have the ability
to adopt a novel behavior based on their prior repertoire of
tool-use skills, and that newly
developed tool-use behaviors can diffuse to other members of a
community.
FRANS DE WAAL
NATIONAL PRIMATE RESEARCH CENTER, EMORY UNIVERSITY, USA
Email: dewaal@emory.edu
Prosocial Primates: The Altruism Question
Evolutionary theory postulates that altruistic behavior evolved
for the return-benefits it bears the
performer. For return-benefits to play a motivational role,
however, they need to be experienced by the organism. Motivational analyses should restrict themselves,
therefore, to the altruistic
impulse and its knowable consequences. Empathy is an ideal
candidate mechanism to underlie
so-called "directed altruism," i.e. altruism in response
to another's pain, need, or distress. The
possibility that animals have empathy and sympathy has received
little systematic attention,
however, due to an excessive fear of anthropomorphism and a taboo
on animal emotions. Actual
animal behavior, however, would lead one to agree with Charles
Darwin that "Many animals
certainly sympathize with each other's distress or danger."
In my own work with monkeys and
apes, I have found many cases of one individual coming to
another's rescue in a fight, putting an
arm around a previous victim of attack, or other emotional
responses to the distress of others.
Empathy has many levels, from basic perception-action mechanisms
(probably related to mirror
neurons) to ever greater cognitive elaborations that include
perspective-taking. The basic forms
probably exist in all mammals as they serve important survival
functions for animals with
vulnerable young. The higher forms of empathy require a sharp
self-other distinction found only
in humans over the age of two, and a few other large-brained
species: apes, dolphins, and
elephants. Perception of the emotional state of another
automatically activates shared
representations causing a matching emotional state in the
observer. With increasing cognition,
state-matching evolved into more complex forms, including concern
for the other and
perspective-taking. Empathy-induced altruism derives its strength
from the emotional stake it
offers the self in the other's welfare. The dynamics of the
empathy mechanism agree with
predictions from kin selection and reciprocal altruism theory.
POSTER PRESENTATIONS
1. Why regurgitation and reingestion occurs in captive lowland
gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)
Kazuki TAKARADA1, Mitsunori NAGAO2, Hiroe
KAMANARU2, Yuuki YAMAMOTO2, Masayuki TANAKA3
1Faculty of Science, Kyoto University; 2Kyoto City Zoo;
3Wildlife
Research Center, Kyoto University
2. Capuchin monkeys are sensitive to others' labor: an analysis of
experimentally induced reward- sharing
behavior
Ayaka TAKIMOTO1,2, Kazuo FUJITA1
1Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University; 2Japan Society for
the Promotion of Science
3. Emotion recognition in capuchin monkeys
Yo MORIMOTO1,2, Kazuo FUJITA1
1Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University; 2Japan Society for
the Promotion of Science
4. Dogs' understanding of the human pointing gesture
Akiko TAKAOKA, Kazuo FUJITA
Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University
5. Dominance of relative judgment in Japanese people
Sota WATANABE1,2, Noriyuki NAKAMURA2,3, Kazuo
FUJITA1
1Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University; 2Japan Society for
the Promotion of Science; 3Faculty of Letters,
Chiba University
6. Contrafreeloading for a movie reward in Japanese macaques
Tadatoshi OGURA
Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Japan Society for
the Promotion of Science
7. Discriminative learning by the conspecific’s behavioral
stimuli in pigeons (Clumba livia) with introducing
different topographies and reinforcers
Akiho MURAMATSU1, Kenichi FUJI2
1Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University; 2College of
Letters, Ritsumeikan University
8. Concurrent acquisition of premises and the symbolic distance
effect in transitive inference task in rats
Sumie IWASAKI1, Tohru TANIUCHI2
1Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University; 2Faculty of Human
Sciences, Kanazawa University
9. The genotypes of coat color gene may predict the successful
training of drug detection dog
Hisayo KISHI, Miho INOUE-MURAYAMA
Wildlife Research Center of Kyoto University
10. Perceptual and motor development of dog puppies (Canis
familiaris): longitudinal observation using a
visual cliff
Tomomi MAEDA1, Ayako MORISAKI2, and Kazuo
FUJITA1
1Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University, 2Kokoro Research
Center, Kyoto University
11. Development of infant carrying in Japanese macaques
Kazunori YAMADA
Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University
12. Gene Polymorphisms in Japanese Akita Dog and Its Relation to
Personality
Akitsugu KONNO1, Miho INOUE-MURAYAMA2, Toshikazu
HASEGAWA1
1Department of Cognitive and Behavioral Science, The University of
Tokyo; 2Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto
University
13. Searching personality-related genes in elephants in Japanese
zoos
Saki YASUI1, Akitsugu KONNO2, Masayuki
TANAKA1, Gen'ichi IDANI1,
Miho INOUE-MURAYAMA1
1Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University; 2Graduate School of
Art and Science, University of Tokyo
14. Habitat use of bush hyraxes in Mahale
Erika IIDA
Wildlife Research Center, Kyoto University
15. Spatial representation of Syrian hamsters (mesocricetus
auratus): The effect of the relation of positions
between goals and various cue-objects for them
Tomoyuki TAMAI1, Toru BETSUYAKU1, Noriyuki
NAKAMURA2,3, Kazuo FUJITA1
1Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University; 2Faculty of Letters
Chiba University; 3Japan Society for the
Promotion of Science
16. Jungle crows (Corvus macrorhynchos) show retrospective but not
prospective metamemory in delayed
matching-to-sample
Kazuhiro GOTO1, Shigeru WATANABE2
1Kokoro Research Center, Kyoto University; 2Faculty of Letters,
Keio University
17. Laterality of manual actions in nut cracking by captive tufted
capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
Yoshiaki SATO1,2, Yui FUJIMORI3, Misato
HAYASHI1
1Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University; 2Japan Society for
the Promotion of Science; 3Faculty of Applied
Biological Sciences, Gifu University
18. Do Syrian hamsters flexibly use beacon and geometric cues in a
spatial navigation task?
Toru BETSUYAKU1, Noriyuki NAKAMURA2,3, Kazuo
FUJITA1
1Kyoto University; 2Chiba University; 3JSPS
19. Working memory of numerals in human children
Sana INOUE
Great Ape Research Institute, Hayashibara Biochemical
Laboratories, Inc.
INFORMATION
Kyoto University Clock Tower Centenary Hall (on the main campus)
Address: Yoshida-Honmachi, Sakyo-ku, Kyoto, 606-8501
http://www.kyoto-u.ac.jp/en/clocktower
Conference will be held in International Conference Hall I
(c) 2010 Primate Research Institute
Front page photograph by Susana Carvalho
Booklet design by Kim Hockings, Susana Carvalho & Paulo
Fuentez
|